Skip to main content

Full text of "From Phonology to Discourse: Studies in Six Colombian Languages"

See other formats


Coreguaje: Tone, Stress, and 
Intonation 



Frances L. Gralow 



0. Introduction 

Coreguaje' has a complicated suprasegmental system which, for a 
number of years, caused much confusion to linguists studying the 
language, who vacillated between calling it a multiple stress system and 
a tonal system. (Of the other languages in the Western Tucanoan 
family, some have been analyzed as stress languages and others as tone 
languages.) 

A major reason for the difficulty is the fact that pitch differences, as 
well as stress differences, are very slight and very difficult to hear. 
This is especially true in men's speech. Because mostly men were 
available as language consultants, the first problem was hearing the 
language well enough to record it correctly. 

According to Eunice V. Pike, *' When high pitch is a feature of a tone 
system, the syllable with high tone is frequently shorter than a syllable 
with low tone.. .in analogous environments'' (1974:169). Since it is true 
that high, stressed syllables in Coreguaje tend to be shorter in duration 
than low, stressed syllables, this argued for a tone system. Eunice Pike 
also stated, ''When high pitch is a contrastive feature of stress, vowel 
quality is frequently conditioned by distribution into that stressed 
syllable versus distribution into a nonhigh syllable. On the other hand, 
when high pitch is a feature of a tone system, vowel quality is 
unaffected by distribution into a high versus a nonhigh syllable'' (1974: 
171). There is no conditioning in the quality of Coreguaje phonemes 
which would correlate with the occurrence of high pitch on a syllable. 
This was further evidence for a tone system. 

Coreguaje nouns, the majority of which are disyllabic with the 
pattern CVCV, present a problem because of the fact that in isolation 
generally there is no contrast in either stress or pitch. In the end we 
found that in certain frames there were four contrasting sets, but in 
isolation phrase stress completely neutralized the contrasts, at least in 
CVCV nouns. 



4 Gralow 

In the process of the analysis it finally became evident that stress 
was basically predictable, though tone was not. When no consistent 
tone sandhi patterns were discovered, the function of intonation was 
considered. It was then found that Coreguaje has contrastive intonation 
patterns which affect and even override word tone patterns, making the 
latter very difficult to distinguish. 

1. Stress and Rhythm 

Rhythm is an important consideration in Coreguaje phonology. Word 
length, stress, and intonation all work together to affect Coreguaje 
rhythm. (Intonation is discussed in section 3.) 

Every phonological word, sentence-level phrase, and sentence in 
Coreguaje has a tendency to fade away; that is, as a unit progresses, 
the syllables tend to become lower in pitch, shorter in length, and 
diminished in volume. The first syllable of a stem of a word is usually 
stressed. Stress is characterized by perceived loudness and length. 
High pitches tend to be higher toward the beginning of the word, 
phrase, or sentence than they are toward the end. (For a slight 
deviation from this pattern, see sect. 3.1.) Also, the final syllable or 
syllables of independent verbs tend to become voiceless before pauses. 

The length of the word, phrase, or sentence is very important in 
Coreguaje phonology. The decrescendo effect is most prominent in 
longer units. Word length is also important in tone rules as well as in 
morphophonemic rules, 

2. Tone 

There are two tone levels in Coreguaje, high and low. (High tone is 
written ' and low tone is unmarked.) Words of two syllables contrast in 
all four possible sequences, e.g., high-high, hdma 'deer'; high-low, 
ndso 'monkeys'; low-high, rut^a 'flies'; and low-low, wekH 'tapirs'. 
These contrasts are found in the following contexts: (1) phrase initially 
on words which are not in the phrase nucleus, or (2) phrase medially 
on words which occur either following words where there is no 
perturbation across word boundaries or following words ending in low 
tone and which are in the same close-knit sequence.^ (For further 
discussion on phrases, see sect. 3.) 

All single morpheme verb stems and the majority of noun stems 
consist of two syllables. Syllables are of the following types: 

First syllable: Vi, CVi, or CVi'? 
Second syllable: V2, or CV2 
'^ = glottal stop 



Coreguaje 



C = all consonants except "^ 

Vi = all oral and nasalized vowels 

V2 - all oral vowels^ 



Examples: 






*to drink' 
'fire' 
'manioc' 
'possum' 



Tone rules vary slightly between stems of different syllable patterns. 
The three stem types are (C)VCV, (OV^CV, and (C)VV. 
Examples: 



jpwa 
hua 



'to enter' 

'to give advice' 

'to pierce' 



Stems of the first two patterns function essentially alike, except that 
syllables with glottal stop tend to be higher in pitch than high tone 
syllables without it. To date, no stems of the pattern (C)VV have been 
found with the tone pattern low-high. ((C)VV stems are considered to 
be two syllables.) 



2,1- Nouns. The four contrasting tone patterns mentioned are partially 
or completely neutralized when two-syllable nouns are pronounced in 
isolation, e.g., as a one-word statement in response to the question 
"What are they?" — hama 'Deer/ — or as a one-word clarification 
question in response to a statement or question such as "(Did you say) 
deer?" Basic tone patterns were determined by listening to nouns in 

such close-knit phrases as pa'^imey 'There are ' and short 

sentences such as re haamijPi '1 see a ' 

Nouns with the pattern (C)VCV show neutralization of tone when 
pronounced in isolation. (In the following examples, * indicates stress 
on the following syllable, ' indicates high pitch, a indicates high falling 
pitch, and ' indicates low pitch.) 



Basic Form 


Statement 


Question 


*hama 'deer' 


*hama 'Deer.' 


*hama 'Deer?' 


^ndsd 'monkeys' 


*nasd 'Monkeys. 


' *ndsd 'Monkeys?' 


*rut^a 'flies' 


*rut^a 'Flies.' 


W4t^a 'Flies?' 


*w€kH 'tapirs' 


^\fekH 'Tapirs.' 


\\^€k^i 'Tapirs?' 



6 Gralow 

Those nouns with the pattern (C)VV, however, do contrast 
somewhat in isolation as a one-word statement. 



Basic Form 


Statement 


Question 


Wie 'anteaters' 


*mie 'Anteaters/ 


Wie 'Anteaters?' 


^sii) 'pheasants' 


'a7// Theasants/ 


KsiTi * Pheasants?' 


^t^eci 'a bird' 


't^^a ^A bird/ 


7V« ^A bird?^ 



2.2. Verbs. In this section the tone patterns of independent verbs in 
isolation are presented/ Since Coreguaje is a verb-heavy language, 
sentences consisting of only an independent verb are very common. 
Since tone is not conditioned across pauses, there is a relatively 
uncomplicated body of data for consideration. 

An independent verb must have at least one high tone and one low 
tone. It cannot have more than three high tones. When two high tones 
occur on contiguous syllables, there is some free variation in the actual 
pitches so that one or the other may be slightly higher, or they may be 
the same. When three high tones occur on contiguous syllables, if the 
first or the last high-tone syllable occurs contiguous to a low-tone 
syllable, it will usually be lowered to a mid. A very few suffixes have 
inherent high tone. Sequences of tones on the independent verb are 
determined by the interrelationships of the tones of the stem, the tones 
of the suffixes, and the total number of syllables in the suffix cluster. 

The following rules apply to independent verbs. Examples are 
provided after each rule. Note that stems of the tone pattern high-low 
often act differently from the others. (Here high tone is marked 'and 
low tone is unmarked.) 

Rule 1. The first syllable of a suffix or suffix cluster bears high tone. 
Examples: 



Sill 

go 



-I- -mi 

masc sing 



Sill mi 

'he is going' 



jawe + 

be hidden 



intent 



-I- -mi 

masc sing 



jaweha'^mi 
'it will be hidden' 



hore -f -si -h -na -h - '-^ + -me = horesina '^me 

lie complete pi anim nom stat pi 'they lied' 



kahe H- -si'^ + -kH 



-H -a^ -\~ -mi 



= kah es i ^/c ^ia '^mi 



descend complete masc sg nom stat masc sg 'he descended' 



asa -+■ -repa + -ma '^ -H -mi 
hear truly neg masc sg 



= asarepama ^mi 

'he truly did not hear' 



Coreguaje 7 

Rule 2. The last syllable of the independent verb cannot bear high tone 
except when the suffix is only one syllable long and the verb 
components fulfill one of the following conditions. 

a. The stem is of the (C)VV syllable pattern and the last syllable of 
the stem is low. Examples: 

qi + -mi = qimi 

eat masc sing 'he is eating' 

peo + -kH = peokH 

be not neuter interr Ms there not any?' 

k^oo + -mf = k^oomi 

receive masc sing 'he receives' 

b. The suffix has inherent high tone. Examples: 

jo'^o -\- 'kH = jo'^okH 

work masc sing interr Ms he working?' 

mit^d -^ 'kH = mk^akH 

come masc sing interr 'is he coming?' 



asa 
hear 



+ -i 



masc sg compi interr 



asm 



'did he hear?' 



If neither of the above conditions is present, the penultimate syllable 
of the independent verb with a suffix cluster of two syllables bears high 
tone and the ultimate syllable bears low tone. Examples: 









Rule 2 


masi + -ma '^ 


+ 


-kH = *masima'^kH 


masimd^kH 


know neg 




masc sg 
interr 


'doesn't he know? 


jawe 


+ 


-mi = '^jdwemi 


j awe mi 


be hidden 




masc sg 


'it is hidden' 


kahe 


+ 


-mi = kdhemi 


kdhemi 


descend 




masc sg 


'he descends' 



Rule 3. When there are four or more syllables in the suffix cluster, 
Rule 2, of course, does not apply. If the stem is of the pattern high- 
low, a high tone occurs on the second syllable of the suffix cluster in 



8 Gralow 

addition to the high tone on the first syllable (from Rule 1). Suffix 
clusters following all other stems have a high tone only on the first 
syllable of the suffix cluster, when all the suffixes are inherently low. 
The tones of the stem remain the same as its basic form in these cases, 
though the second syllable of a low-low stem may vary to mid. 
Examples: 

jo'^o + -repasina'^me = "^o'^orepasina'^me *they truly worked' 

work they truly did *jd'^orepasina'^me Rule I 

jd'^orepasina'^me Rule 3 

k^oo -f -si'^kNa'^mi = *k^dosi'^k'*ia'^mi *he received' 

receive he-completive *k^dosi'^kHa '^mi Rule I 

k^oosV^kHa^^mi Rule 3 

asa + -si'^kHa'^mi = *asasi'^kHa'^mi 'he heard' 

hear he-completive asast'^kHa '^mi Rule 1 

Two-syllable suffixes which have inherent high tone tend to act 
differently from suffixes without inherent high tone. They also seem to 
have stress on the first syllable as do stems. Historically these high 
tone suffixes were probably verb stems, but because of their 
dependence on other verbs they now function as suffixes. These 
suffixes are: 

-k'^ai benefactive 

-hai accompanying movement 

-koso probability 

When used in an independent verb these suffixes keep their inherent 
tones, and the basic tones of the stem do not change. 

The suffix -kua behaves irregularly. When following a high-high 
stem, the stem tones become high-low and -kua bears low tone. All 
other stems become low-high and -kua is high-low. Suffixes following 
-kua follow Rules 1-3. 

3. Intonation 

Two types of intonation contours have been observed in Coreguaje 
speech: those covering a phrase (or sentence portion or clause), after 
which there is a tentative pause, and those preceding a final pause. 

(1) In nonfinal contours, there is an optional premargin in which 
basic word tones of words are maintained, though the pitch levels are 
usually quite close together. The nucleus of this pause group (defined 
as the syllable containing the loudest stress and longest vowel length) 
occurs on the stressed syllable of the last word in the group, i.e., on 



Coreguaje 9 

the last stressed syllable in the pause group. The final pitch of this unit 
is predictable in accord with the tones of the final word or words as 
follows: 

If the final word has the tones high-high or low-high, there is no 
change in pitch. If it is high-low, the low tone is raised to just slightly 
lower than the high syllable, so that the word is pronounced high-mid. 
If the word preceding a high-high final word in this group ends in low 
tone, the intonation generally follows the tone patterns fairly closely. If 
the penultimate word ends in a syllable bearing basic high tone, that 
high tone may remain high, or the pitch may rise gradually from the 
beginning of the penultimate word to the high of the nucleus, thus 
giving the last syllable of the penultimate word a mid pitch. 

9cai t\yi_ 'p^i^t^ 'what is in the body' 
body being, life 



!jdi^ ^haaW) 'when the witch doctor looks' 

witch doctor when looks 



*fear in one's heart' 




or 



(2) In final contours there is a strong down-glide of pitch beginning 
with the nucleus of the word in the phrase nucleus and continuing over 
the rest of the syllables in the phonological phrase. The nucleus of this 
phrase is usually on the nucleus of the independent verb, but may also 
occur on question words (which are at the beginning of the phrase) or 
on any word which receives emphasis. All high tones occurring after 
the phrase nucleus in the final contour are considerably lowered. 



'noo/m^^^-Jro/ 'the witch doctor reveals' 

he shows witch doctor 

In addition, there is an optional postmargin to this main final phrase 
which may be a sort of tag comment in the form of a dependent clause 
or a repetition of one of the items in the main clause, such as the 
subject or object. All words in this final margin are perceptually very 



10 Gralow 

low, though at times a very sHght rise is heard on basic high tone 
syllables. The final word of this postmargin has an even lov^er down- 
glide. 



^ukHna 




witch doctors they-know banisterium those who drink 
'witch doctors know (those who drink banisterium)' 

4. Summary 

Coreguaje phonology is characterized by a strong decrescendo effect 
over phrases, sentences, and longer words. This tends to minimize 
contrasts between high and low tone, depending on where in the pause 
group words occur. Nonfinal intonation allows phrase-final high tones 
to remain high, but pulls corresponding low tones to mid. Final 
intonation causes high tones to be lowered, often to the point of being 
perceived as low. Otherwise, noun and verb stems of two syllables 
contrast in all four possible sequences of tone. Suffixes with inherent 
high tone generally keep those tones. Suffixes with no inherent high 
tones may receive one or more high tones depending on the tone 
pattern of the stem and the total number of syllables in the word. 



Notes 

1 Coreguaje is a member of the Western Tucanoan language family and is spoken by 
approximately 800 people who live on the tributaries of the Orteguaza River below 
Florencia and the Caqueta River below Solano, in Caqueta, Colombia. Research for this 
paper was done under the auspices of the Summer Institute of Linguistics between 
February 1975 and August 1979. Most of the present analysis was developed during a 
linguistic workshop conducted by Dr. Donald A. Burquest of the University of Texas at 
Arlington in August 1978 at the Summer Institute of Linguistics center in Lomalinda, 
Colombia. It was then revised during a phonology workshop conducted by Dr. Ruth 
Brend of Michigan State University in February 1981, also at Lomalinda. The principal 
Coreguaje language consultants in this project were Alicia Garcia Pizarro and Silvio 
Fajardo from Maticuru and Elicenia Gutierrez Gasca from Jacome. 

2 In close-knit verb phrases where the first word of the phrase is an adverb, a verb stem, 
or an incorporated noun, the first syllable of the following conjugated verb is raised to 
high tone if the first word ends in low-low. 

Examples: 

'q) + Svesim^ = 'q) 'wesimi 'he doesn't know how to eat' 

eat he doesn't know 

'ho'^e + 'ra)mi = 'ho'^e 'ra)m^ 'he's coming again' 

again he comes 

hva'^) + 'sa)mi = Sva'^} 'satmi 'he's going hunting' 

meat he goes 



Coreguaje 1 1 

The only other perturbation occurring other than that conditioned by intonation is the 
phenomenon of either a high tone or a low tone optionally lowering or raising to mid 
when occurring between a low and a high. 

3 If Vi is not the same as V2 (disregarding nasalization), they cannot both be rounded 
vowels. To avoid this combination, u becomes f in the environment of o, 

4 This section is included in outline form in the article by Young, Cook, and Gralow 
about Coreguaje phonology. 



References 

Pike, Eunice V. 1974. ''A Multiple Stress System Versus a Tone 

System." UAL 40:3:169-75. 
Young, Carolyn M., Dorothy Cook, and Frances Gralow. "Informe 

Descriptivo de los Fonemas del Coreguaje." Sistemas Fonologicos 

de Idiomas Colombianos 5. 



LANGUAGE DATA 
Amerindian Series No. 9 

From Phonology to Discourse: 

Studies in Six Colombian Languages 

Ruth M. Brend 

Editor 

Summer Institute of Linguistics 

7500 W. Camp Wisdom Road 

Dallas, Texas 

1985 

Editor-in-chief Desmond Derbyshire 

Asian- Pacific Series Phyllis Healey 
David Thomas 
African Series Pamela Bendor-Samuel 

Amerindian Series Viola Waterhouse 

©1985 by the Summer Institute of Linguistics 
ISBN 0-88312-091-7